The creature鈥檚 tiny but remarkably wrinkled brain supports the idea that brain complexity can evolve before brain size in the primate family tree.
The ancient monkey, known scientifically as Victoriapithecus, first made headlines in 1997 when its fossilized skull was discovered on an island in Kenya鈥檚 Lake Victoria, where it lived 15 million years ago.
Researchers using high-resolution X-ray imaging have peered inside its cranial cavity and created a three-dimensional computer model of what the animal鈥檚 brain likely looked like.
Micro-CT scans of the creature鈥檚 skull show that Victoriapithecus had a tiny brain relative to its body.
Coauthors Fred Spoor of the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology and Lauren Gonzales of Duke University calculated its brain volume to be about 36 cubic centimeters, which is less than half the volume of monkeys of the same body size living today.
If similar-sized monkeys have brains the size of oranges, the brain of this particular male was more akin to a plum.
鈥淲hen Lauren finished analyzing the scans she called me and said, 鈥榊ou won鈥檛 believe what the brain looks like,鈥欌 said coauthor Brenda Benefit, a professor of anthropology in NMSU鈥檚 College of Arts and Sciences who first discovered the skull with NMSU coauthor Monte McCrossin, associate professor of anthropology.
Despite its puny proportions, the animal鈥檚 brain was surprisingly complex. The CT scans revealed numerous distinctive wrinkles and folds, and the olfactory bulb -- the part of the brain used to perceive and analyze smells -- was three times larger than expected.
鈥淚t probably had a better sense of smell than many monkeys and apes living today,鈥 said Gonzales, an NMSU alum studying for her doctorate degree at Duke. 鈥淚n living higher primates you find the opposite: the brain is very big, and the olfactory bulb is very small, presumably because as their vision got better their sense of smell got worse. But instead of a tradeoff between smell and sight, Victoriapithecus might have retained both capabilities.鈥
The findings, published in the July 3 issue of Nature Communications, are important because they offer new clues to how primate brains changed over time, and during a period from which there are very few fossils.
鈥淭his is the oldest skull researchers have found for Old World monkeys, so it鈥檚 one of the only clues we have to their early brain evolution,鈥 Benefit said.
In the absence of fossil evidence, previous researchers have disagreed over whether primate brains got bigger first, and then more folded and complex, or vice versa.
鈥淚n the part of the primate family tree that includes apes and humans, the thinking is that brains got bigger and then they get more folded and complex,鈥 Gonzales said. 鈥淏ut this study is some of the hardest proof that in monkeys, the order of events was reversed -- complexity came first and bigger brains came later.鈥
The findings also lend support to claims that the small brain of the human ancestor Homo floresiensis, whose 18,000-year-old skull was discovered on a remote Indonesian island in 2003, isn鈥檛 as remarkable as it might seem. In spite of their pint-sized brains, Homo floresiensis were able to make fire and use stone tools to kill and butcher large animals.
鈥淏rain size and brain complexity can evolve independently; they don鈥檛 have to evolve together at the same time,鈥 Benefit said.
Benefit and McCrossin excavated the skull during the 1990s from 15 million-year-old Middle Miocene deposits on a small island in Lake Victoria, Kenya.
鈥淭he discovery was very important because the skull looks different from what people had predicted that ancestral monkeys would look like, which in turn changed our concept of what the common ancestor of monkeys and apes would look like,鈥 Benefit said. 鈥淚t was truly fun. It was fulfilling a long held dream.鈥
At the time of the excavation, Benefit and McCrossin couldn鈥檛 get a good glimpse of the inside of the skull, and estimated the brain鈥檚 volume was small but not abnormally small, Benefit said.
It was only a few years ago that the micro-CT scans by Spoor allowed Gonzales, then a graduate student at NMSU, and Spoor to get a better sense of the brains鈥 volume.
Improved scans 鈥渁llowed (Spoor) and Lauren to use the impressions the brain had made inside of the skull, which thankfully were fairly deep and clear in this case, to extract the brain from the skull,鈥 Benefit said. 鈥淟auren and Fred were actually able to use a 3D printer to make a solid printout of the brain.鈥
The work was funded by the Max Planck Society and University College London. The skull was originally discovered with support from the National Science foundation.
MEDIA CONTACT
Register for reporter access to contact detailsArticle Multimedia
CITATIONS
Nature Communications